Sunday, April 24, 2016

How should you study for an exam? Does it matter if the test is essay or multiple choice?

As the academic trend of more and more standardized testing continues, many students have started to look for study tips to make the process less painful. And while there may be some unique tricks, the perhaps best-known ones are also the ones usually under-utilized. The two simple habits of getting a good night’s rest before an exam and quizzing yourself over the material are the most reliable ways to ensure better test performance.

To begin, good test performance isn’t just about what your brain is doing during wake, but also the consolidation and reorganization of memories that happens during sleep. In 2006, Walker and Stickgold published a review of the evidence demonstrating that during sleep, memories are more deeply encoded such that they can be retrieved at a later time. Research supports the hypothesis that during sleep, memories of the day are transformed into a deeper encoded form, which can be more easily accessed and integrated into other previous knowledge. This ability to contextualize new information within previous experiences or knowledge allows for incorporated networks that can help us address multi-faceted problems, such as when taking an essay exam. Additionally, a lack of sleep can actually be detrimental not only because studied material isn’t assimilated as well, but also because sleep deprivation leads to difficulties with attention. Both total sleep deprivation and sleep restriction affect the ability to maintain vigilant attention, a skill absolutely necessary for any sort of exam (Lim & Dinges, 2008). Thus adequate sleep before an exam prevents disrupting mind wandering and has the added benefit of helping consolidate previously studied information.
 
Me in philosophy class freshman year- thank god it wasn't during an exam...
Another strategy for preparing for exams is to use the practice effect. Psychologists have recognized the practice effect for decades now, demonstrating that practicing testing or quizzing yourself improves your ability to call that information to mind later (Sutterer & Awh, 2015). Essentially, when studying it is in best practice to constantly test yourself, or have others make up test questions for you. Beyond the obvious benefit of realizing which subjects aren’t as well engrained, research has shown that testing helps make memories more accessible to be called upon later. This technique can be used both for essay tests (by outlining responses to potential prompts) and for multiple-choice exams. Although the practice effect might require taking some time to meet with a study partner or group and quiz each other, the benefit of such a habit will certainly lead to improved testing scores.

By combining the practice effect with a sufficient night’s sleep, students can better prepare for all sorts of exams they may experience in their academic careers. In the case of these techniques, it doesn’t matter what kind of exam is given, or what material is covered. Although study habit can be difficult to form, sleep and self-testing will lead to a better performance than could be achieved by staying up all night to cram by re-reading notes.


Works Cited
Lim, J., & Dinges, D. F. (2008). Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences1129(1), 305-322.

Sutterer, D. W., & Awh, E. (2015). Retrieval practice enhances the accessibility but not the quality of memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1-11.


Walker, M. P., & Stickgold, R. (2006). Sleep, memory, and plasticity. Annu. Rev. Psychol.57, 139-166.

Monday, April 11, 2016

Why do we forget?

Thanks to brain training companies, it seems everyone is trying to increase his or her memory. It’s easy to argue that better memory can only positively impact your life, but in fact forgetting serves an equally important role in cognition. Specifically, forgetting is necessary for two reasons- forgetting (especially of details) allows for automatized skills and semantic encoding and forgetting is emotionally adaptive to neglect some painful or negative memories.
First let us examine the case of AJ, a woman with highly superior autobiographic memory. If you were to ask AJ what happened 20 years ago on April 11, she could list exactly what she wore that day, along with all of her activities. Her memory is able to retain an episodic account of nearly every day of her life. However, this ability deeply intrudes on her life. She reports, “Most have called it a gift but I call it a burden. I run my entire life through my head every day and it drives me crazy” (Parker et al., 2006, p. 35). Imagine every time you went to the grocery store, you remembered every other instance of a trip to buy milk. It would be cognitively taxing and unnecessary to sustain all these individual memories. And in fact, a study by Logan and Crump (2009) demonstrated the necessity of losing detailed memory in professional typists, who aren’t able to verbalize where keys are located or how their hands are moving. Yet these workers are still extremely proficient in their job, and when they are asked to explicitly focus on what their hands are doing, their performance suffers. These typists do not have detailed memory of every time they sat down at a keyboard, but their skill demonstrates that a composite of all those experiences meshed together have given them their proficiency. Thus, forgetting may allow for details of episodes to be removed so that abstraction and generalized skill learning can occur.
As mentioned before, forgetting not only benefits cognition, but also assists in emotional stability. Joorman and colleagues (2009), among many others, have demonstrated that depression is linked to an inability to forget negative memories. Perhaps if we were able to remember every embarrassing moment and social faux pas, we would be less likely to take social risks or even engage with others at all. Forgetting negative experiences can help us stay open to new social experiences or maintain important relationships. Going back to the case of AJ, she was diagnosed with both depression and anxiety, maybe because she couldn’t forget her negative experiences. Thus, AJ exemplifies that there is no evolutionary benefit to remembering every negative experience.
Memory is unequivocally important for many functions, but forgetting also serves a purpose in maintaining healthy functioning. Without forgetting, we might be less positive about the future and unwilling to take the big risks that advance science and other fields. We would also be unable to garner abstract information from multiple situations or sources we encounter. Like tow sides of the same coin, forgetting and remembering balance each other.




Works Cited

Joormann, J., Hertel, P. T., LeMoult, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Training forgetting of negative material in depression. Journal of abnormal psychology118(1), 34.

Logan, G. D., & Crump, M. J. (2009). The Left Hand Doesn't Know What the Right Hand Is Doing The Disruptive Effects of Attention to the Hands in Skilled Typewriting. Psychological Science20(10), 1296-1300.

Nørby, S. (2015). Why Forget? On the Adaptive Value of Memory Loss.Perspectives on Psychological Science10(5), 551-578.

Parker, E. S., Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (2006). A case of unusual autobiographical

            remembering. Neurocase12(1), 35-49.